Motor skills and abilities
Since the speed at which individuals develop motor skills and abilities can vary greatly, it is almost impossible to put a label on when each step in this development will be completed. It is important to distinguish between skills and abilities in this context. Skills are visible patterns of movement that are performed consciously and deliberately. ?e basic forms of movement include sitting, standing, gripping, running, and jumping, and are learned at a very young age. Especially in their first year of life, a child will acquire an astounding number of new gross and fine motor skills. Each of these skills will improve and become more clearly defined with time, until the child reaches their peak motor activity level at around seven or eight years old. Once this level has been attained, a process of individualization begins. The course this takes can vary greatly, ranging from stagnation and negligible development to very dynamic development of motor performance.
Motor abilities, on the other hand, are the control and functional processes that form the basis for our motions and positions. These include physiological traits such as stamina and strength, but most importantly also cover all of the factors that affect our sensory performance, perception, cognitive abilities and motivation. For example, while the act of throwing an object at a target requires a certain amount of strength, it also requires the ability to judge distance and throwing technique. As such, the corresponding motor abilities cannot be acquired until he other areas of development have reached the necessary level. One example of the complex interplay between different functions is in visual motor ability, where visual information is used to control movement. This ability improves as a child gets older, thus allowing the child to perform the corresponding movements faster, more reliably and with greater precision. Another example is the sense of movement within one's body, or coenesthesia. This term refers to a person's awareness of their own position in a space, which does not develop until between the ages of six and twelve.
Regulating one's balance is also an activity that requires several of our body's functions to work together, which is why smaller children have difficulty staying balanced with their eyes closed, as they rely primarily on visual information for their orientation. As the body matures, vision becomes less important and is superseded by the use of coenesthesia.
One of the largest risk factors in interactions with traffic is the small size of children's bodies. This makes it harder for the children themselves to see past obstacles, and also prevents other road users from seeing them easily.
A sense of balance is most important when it comes to riding a bicycle. The problem in this scenario is caused by the fact that a child's head is quite large in proportion to the rest of their body, which makes hard for them to find their balance. In terms of motor skills and abilities, children should possess the skills required to ride a bicycle by around the age of ten. In order to cycle safely on the road, however, they must be able to utilize a huge number of more complex motion and cognitive processes that involve different functions working together. Children will not possess the appropriate abilities until they are around 14. Due to the intrinsic developments they undergo during puberty, however, they are also more likely to take risks and overestimate their own abilities at this age, which once more increases the risk of accidents.
Cognitive abilities
One of the most elementary cognitive abilities is attention. In the first years of a child's life, this is primarily controlled reflexively – the child simply reacts to external visual or acoustic stimuli. It is not until between the ages of five and eleven that they develop the ability to direct their attention with focus and deliberate intent. This ability reaches adult levels around the age of 14. This ability is extremely important for children in terms of road safety, as they will not possess cognitive control of their own behavior unless they can actually direct their attention toward the traffic around them. As soon as they are distracted, the link to their memory – and thus to their knowledge of how traffic works, the traffic regulations, how to behave, and risk awareness – is lost. Correspondingly, the risk of an accident becomes very high. The phenomenon of distraction persists into puberty. It is a similar story with divided attention, or the ability to pay attention to two or more requirements at the same time. Children especially have difficulty with this when the tasks do not all have the same priority.
Risk awareness
Risk awareness develops in three stages, starting from the age of six. The first stage is the development of an acute risk awareness, whereby a risk is not detected until the actual moment of danger, sometimes leaving little scope to act. Next, at around the age of eight, children develop anticipation risk awareness, meaning they begin to be able to recognize potential hazards as dangerous in advance. Children in this phase can alter or even completely avoid the dangerous situation by taking alternative action. In the last phase, which begins at around nine or ten, children develop preventive risk awareness, which enables them to avoid hazards before they occur. As a restriction, it should be noted that from the anticipation risk awareness phase onward, a child cannot assess risks adequately based solely on their own experiences with traffic – they also require other sources of information, particularly knowledge of the risks associated with particular types of traffic.
This is made more difficult by the fact that risk awareness can fluctuate greatly depending on the situation at hand. Particularly when playing, younger children feel much safer that they actually are in the real traffic situation they are in (high subjective sense of safety combined with low objective safety). In puberty, children are able to recognize risks, but they consciously ignore them or even gravitate toward them during risky cycling maneuvers, when running across the street, or when acting out dares.